Skip to main content

I love music. What about you?  And when I hear a tune, I just want to groove. Admittedly, I’ve never really  given much thought as to why I’m so compelled to move when I hear a beat.   Well, here’s a fun fact post with a scientific explanation. According to researchers at Concordia the pleasurable urge to move to music — to groove — is  actually a physiological response independent of how much we generally enjoy music.  

The researchers say we’re all made to move, even if we consciously don’t like music.   Now isn’t that interesting? It also explains why even those who never want to dance at weddings or parties, somehow manage to groove.

Music, pleasure and the urge to groove

Isaac Romkey, is a PhD student in the Department of Psychology. He writes in the journal PLOS One that recent research shows the two aspects of groove, pleasure and urge to move, while usually closely correlated, may in fact be separable.

“For most of history, humans have listened to and created music. We use music for various reasons, including mood regulation, social connection, and dancing. And one of the more intriguing features of listening to music is that it is often accompanied by a pleasurable urge to move along. In music cognition literature, this pleasurable urge to move to music has been termed “groove” .

Here’s the low down on their research

To test their theory, Romkey and his co-authors compared groove responses to more than 50 short pieces of music in people with musical anhedonia and non-anhedonic controls.

Participants with musical anhedonia were only included if they derived pleasure from other aspects of life, like food and sex, and if they exhibited appropriate reward responses across other metrics.

Testing pitch an beat

The researchers also ensured participants were not depressed and had intact pitch and beat perception. Participants listened to short pieces of music designed to elicit a groove response and varied in rhythmic complexity. After each piece, they were asked to rate how much pleasure they experienced and how much it made them want to move.

“Normally, we would expect to see an inverted U-shaped response to rhythmic complexity, meaning that we want to move to music that is of medium complex rhythms as opposed to music that is very simple or very complex,” Romkey explains.

Based on this, the authors hypothesized that people with anhedonia would show lower pleasure ratings but preserved urge to move ratings for groovy music.

Isaac Romkey explains, “For those with musical anhedonia, they derive pleasure from the urge to move.”

However, they found no differences in either pleasure or urge to move in anhedonics compared to controls. More importantly, they showed that for people with anhedonia, the urge to move appears to drive their experience of pleasure. 

Pleasure from the urge to move

The researchers say, this suggests that the blunted pleasure sensation found in people with musical anhedonia is compensated by the urge to move.

“In the musical anhedonia group, we expected to see a flattening of that U-shaped curve, but that is not what we saw. That implies that for those with musical anhedonia, they derive pleasure from the urge to move. More generally, it suggests that the urge to move may itself generate pleasure.”

Same response, different sources

The causes of musical anhedonia remain understudied, but Romkey says it appears to be heritable.

He notes that the urge to move has been linked to the dorsal striatum, a part of the brain that is linked to motor functions.  Pleasure on the other hand is more associated with the ventral striatum, which regulates reward, motivation and goal-directed behaviour.

End note

The researcher’s work is not done yet. For future studies, they’re going to look at differences in functional and structural connectivity in the brain between anhedonics and controls in the dorsal and ventral striatum using imaging techniques including MRI and magnetoencephalography.

References

Co-authors include Nicholas Foster, Simone Dalla Bella and Virginia Penhune at Concordia and Tomas Matthews, PhD 21, of Aarhus University.
You can read the cited paper here: “The pleasurable urge to move to music is unchanged in people with musical anhedonia.”

Main Photo by cottonbro studio

Read why music is good for our health.

4 Ways Music Promotes Good Mental Health for the Whole Family

 

 

Gisèle Wertheim Aymes

Gisèle Wertheim Aymes

Gisèle is the owner of the Longevity brand. She is a seasoned media professional and autodidactic. Gisèle has a passion for sharing information on good health. You can follow her @giselewaymes on Twitter and Instagram or read her Linked-In profile for full bio details.

Longevity Live is a digital publisher AND DOES NOT OFFER PERSONAL HEALTH OR MEDICAL ADVICE. IF YOU’RE FACING A MEDICAL EMERGENCY, CALL YOUR LOCAL EMERGENCY SERVICES IMMEDIATELY, OR VISIT THE NEAREST EMERGENCY ROOM OR URGENT CARE CENTER. YOU SHOULD CONSULT YOUR HEALTHCARE PROVIDER BEFORE STARTING ANY NUTRITION, DIET, EXERCISE, FITNESS, MEDICAL, OR WELLNESS PROGRAM.

This content, developed through collaboration with licensed medical professionals and external contributors, including text, graphics, images, and other material contained on the website, apps, newsletter, and products (“Content”), is general in nature and for informational purposes only and does not constitute medical advice; the Content is not intended to be a substitute for professional medical advice, diagnosis, or treatment.

Always consult your doctor or other qualified healthcare provider with any questions you may have regarding a medical condition, procedure, or treatment, whether it is a prescription medication, over-the-counter drug, vitamin, supplement, or herbal alternative.

Longevity Live makes no guarantees about the efficacy or safety of products or treatments described in any of our posts. Any information on supplements, related services and drug information contained in our posts are subject to change and are not intended to cover all possible uses, directions, precautions, warnings, drug interactions, allergic reactions, or adverse effects.

Longevity does not recommend or endorse any specific test, clinician, clinical care provider, product, procedure, opinion, service, or other information that may be mentioned on Longevity’s websites, apps, and Content.

Leave a Reply

error: Content is protected !!